This event was spread over two
days, October 29 and 30, 2009, of the 2009 AECT convention.
The panel session took place
on Friday, October 30, from 3:30 to 4:30 p.m. The panel session
was open to all attendees of the convention.
The working group meeting was
by invitation. It was attended by the panelists only. It took
place the day prior to the panel, i.e., Thursday, October 29,
from 4:30 to 8:00 p.m. Its purpose was for the panelists to get
to know each other's views and to exchange thoughts and experiences
on which those views are based.
The panel and workshop are being
organized and facilitated by:
Jan Visser, President, Learning Development Institute.
The panel will furthermore be
composed of:
Michael Hannafin, Director, Learning and Performance
Support Laboratory, University of Georgia.
Michael Spector, Professor, University of Georgia.
Glen Bull, Head, National Technology Leadership
Coalition.
Brent Wilson, Professor, University of Colorado Denver.
Charles Reigeluth, Professor, Indiana University.
A written contribution is expected
from:
Leon Lederman, Nobel Laureate Physics 1988; Director
Emeritus, Fermi National Accelerator Laboratory and Resident
Scholar, Illinois Mathematics and Science Academy.
On the Web pages for the 2009
AECT convention,
the Presidential Panel Session on 'Building the Scientific
Mind' is decribed as follows:
This session aims at fostering discussion on the desirability
for the design of learning environments to move beyond the primary
concern with competencies to mental states defined at a higher
level of complexity. Such mental states, or mindsets, are typically
defined in terms of a broad mix of interacting competencies,
attitudes, values and metacognitive abilities. Taken together,
these sets of mental faculties determine, in a holistic manner,
our ways of being and acting in the world. Conscious care for
the mind is increasingly required as citizens of the twenty-first
century are expected to interact with complex problem situations,
both professionally and in the day-to-day reality of their lives
as contributing members of their societies and communities. The
scientific mind is one of a variety of mindsets.
Over the past years it has been the object of study and debate
among the panelists. The panel contributes to the session as
panelists take a first cut at presenting their thoughts, concerns,
queries and doubts. The audience will be invited to join in doing
the same.
The Scientific
Mind (TSM) has been a
focus area of inquiry for the Learning Development Institute
since its inception in 1999.
A
concept
paper on The Scientific Mind in Context provides an attempt to capture the essence
of the concept of TSM. While no great changes have been proposed
to the paper since it was written, it is still considered a draft
as it is felt that further elaboration is needed to fully delineate
this complex concept. The paper concludes offering the following
suggestions as a starting point for the description of the multidimensional
character on TSM in terms of 11 dispositions:
The spirit of inquiry.
The spirit of collaboration.
The quest for beauty (harmony,
parsimony, wholeness).
The desire to understand and
do so profoundly.
The creative spirit.
The urge to be critical.
The spirit to transcend.
The spirit of building on prior
knowledge.
The search for unity.
The building of the story of
human knowledge and ability.
The spirit of construction.
Since 2005 several international
colloquia on Building the Scientific Mind (BtSM) have
taken place. They are all documented on the Web pages of the
Learning Development Institute. The documentation in question
can be accessed via the following links:
BtSM2005,
held May 17-20, 2005, in The Hague, Netherlands.
BtSM2007, held May 28-31, 2007, in Vancouver,
BC, Canada.
As a first step in developing
the dialogue, panelists were asked to share with their colleagues
initial questions they wished to serve as inspiration for the
dialogue. Each question is followed by a brief rationale. Below
is the tabulated result of this initial exercise. Contributions
appear in the order in which they came in.
Want to contribute
to the dialogue? Please
go to the Learning
Development Institute on Facebook site and use the 'Discussions'
area. Your input will be greatly appreciated.
#
Author
Question
Rationale
Jan Visser
SITUATED RATIONALE
I am both a theoretical physicist
and a learning scientist, having research, teaching, and policy
development experience in both areas. I also spent a lifetime
thinking about what it means to learn and exploring what is involved
in creating knowledge for deep understanding of the world (the
essence of science). More recently, my interest turned to trying
to figure out what those two things have to do with each other.
As a growing human being and
a developing scientist, I have tremendously benefited from circumstances
that were unique to me, but I believe we all come across such
instances of uniqueness in our lives, if we approach life with
an open mind. For me, such unique occurrences, which became of
significant influence to my development, included privileged
opportunities to interact with
the embodied memories of the
intellectual, creative, spiritual, and experiential heritage
of the past (as for instance laid down in written documents,
oral histories, artifacts, performed music, staged representations,
and images);
dedicated mentors, the living
instances of how those senior to me were (and, for some, still
are) playing their role in the history of humanity's development,
who guided me through their love, teaching, guidance, and critical
questioning of my thoughts and actions;
students who, in turn, I served
as mentor, which allowed them to interact with the memories meanwhile
embodied in me, taking advantage of my connections to the past
and the present and to ask me questions that often propelled
me beyond where I was at the time.
The instances of embodied memory
referred to above are what I call 'mind.'
I posit that mind--embodied memory--is
as much worthy of consideration as the collection of competencies
we habitually put our focus on when thinking about the education
of others from an instructional design perspective. The importance
and relevance of the assumptions underlying our field are firmly
supported by evidence. However, I believe that it's time we get
ready for taking the next step, recognizing that there are reasons
to elevate our thinking to considering the implications of learning
at higher levels of complexity.
In raising issues about the
'scientific mind,' I wish to acknowledge that the scientific
mind is but one of multiple mindsets worthy of consideration.
Other mindsets are possible and deserve equal attention. In fact,
different mindsets often overlap--and interact significantly--in
important areas of concern. Nonetheless, I also believe that
the values embodied in the history of the development of the
sciences are of particular relevance to our time. Thus, I hold
that not only professional scientists, but all human beings,
deserve to be given the best chances to develop their scientific
mind.
01
Jan Visser
As outlined in the description above,
the mind is a multifaceted concept. I am proposing making the
(scientific) mind, rather than a set of separate competencies,
the unit of analysis in pursuing major advances in education
and learning. Is this too tall an order from the perspective
of the design of learning environments?
In In
Search of Memory: The Emergence of a New Science of Mind,
Eric Kandel situates the mind as a concept that has traditionally
been the object of inquiry of philosophy and psychology, but
whose study has more recently come within the purview of the
neurosciences as well. The mind can not be seen, despite Descartes,
as separate from the brain. The biology of the mind makes 'mind'
a much more concrete concept, open to inquiry, even though many
of the tools of investigation may still have to be invented.
It thus seems a good time to change perspective and broaden our
vision of learning. Earlier attempts to do so include the chapter
I contributed to the International Handbook of Lifelong Learning
(2001); a special collaboratively authored issue of Educational
Technology Magazine (March-April 2002); and the euqally collaboratively
authored 2008 publication on Learners in a Changing Learning
Landscape: Reflections from a Dialogue on New Roles and Expectations
(Visser & Visser, Eds. - Springer).
02
Jan Visser
In general, what conditions in the
learning environment condition the emergence and development
of the scientific mind?
The term 'learning environment'
in this question should be interpreted in a very brought sense.
It can mean any specific learning environment, weather formal
and deliberately created. It can also refer to an informal and
spontaneously created environment. Moreover, It could mean the
entire learning landscape in which one exists, made up of the
collection of dynamically interacting opportunities for learning
that one discovers having access to during one's lifelong learning
journey.
03
Jan Visser
Is science education, as we know
it, a primary means towards developing the scientific mind? If
not, or if it leaves to be desired, what should change? Might
science education as we know it be detrimental to developing
the scientific mind? If so, in what ways and why?
This question obviously relates
to the formal learning environment, particularly the school.
In most schools teaching occurs in accordance with set curricula
that are highly compartmentalized in disciplines and sub-disciplines
and that are often the product of prevailing ideologies. The
scientific disciplines are frequently believed to be the major
contributors to shaping the scientific mind, usually with a view
to laying the basis for preparing the new generation of scientists.
Should such belief be taken for granted or rather be challenged?
I believe there are good reasons to challenge it.
04
Jan Visser
Beyond formal science education,
what advice would we have as regards rethinking schooling systems
such that mindful learning, and in particular the development
of the scientific mind, ensue?
The setting for this question is
the same as for the previous question: We are clearly looking
at what happens in schools. Here we should look at potential
opportunities that may be present in the school environment but
that so far have remained unexplored. We may, among other possibilities,
want to look at the presence of disciplines other than science
and their potential contribution to developing the scientific
mind. We may also want to look at possibilities to transgress
and transcend the boundaries between the traditional disciplines.
05
Jan Visser
What informal learning settings
could be particularly interesting for the development of the
scientific mind? What conditions must be satisfied in such environments?
Here again we must think broadly.
Science may or may not be explicitly present in the informal
learning settings we wish to explore. Yet, things may happen
in, say, a conversation around the kitchen table; in an internet
chat room; while reading a book; or when listening to a discussion
on Science Friday, that profoundly change one's way of looking
at and interacting with the world. Many scientists refer to such
circumstances as a key motivation to what turned them on. Is
this a privilege just for scientists or do we all engage with
such opportunities to see the light, in whatever direction it
may propel us professionally, in the sciences or in other domains?
Brent G. Wilson
SITUATED RATIONALE
My questions involve interrogating
the two main constructs of science and mind. Most people think
of science as the objective application of a rigorous method
in pursuit of new knowledge about the (material) world. The method
(which on examination cannot be explicitly defined) is explicitly
materialist and explicitly sidesteps questions of value. Moreover,
the construct of mind suggests a dualism with the material/body.
So both of the primary terms lead to interesting questions.
My bottom line with these questions: I sense a fundamental tension
between science (which is traditionally an effort to subjugate
the material through the mental), mind (necessarily dualistic,
privileging mind over the material), and the more holistic dispositions
that frame the construct in the paper and the website.
06
Brent G. Wilson
How can the scientific mind avoid
the negative aspects of mind-body dualism - e.g., neglect or
oppression of the embodied, the material, and the unnecessary
dividing of mind and body, with the privileging of mind over
body? How are these issues played out in virtual environments,
which are almost by definition disembodied?
Studies of the mind by definition
are dualistic, positing a mental state that corresponds to the
physical (whether neurological or external conditions in the
world). While some insights can be gained from studies of mind,
they can lead to distorted views of things. We either need to
develop models that explicitly acknowledge the material - or
be very careful in our use of dualistic models.
07
Brent G. Wilson
How does a scientific mind embrace
the whole range of human concerns and capacities - for example,
spiritual, aesthetic, emotional/affective, conative/self-regulative,
etc.? Can the study of learning environments benefit from such
broad-based examination and study?
Science is usually thought of in
terms of intellectual knowledge pursuit, but narrowly framed
by materialist assumptions, transcending the personal quirks
and interests of researchers. I sense a sympathy to these concerns
- but how are they compatible with the practice of science -
and the notion of mind? The study of learning environments badly
needs a broader perspective (presently dominated by cognitivist
assumptions) - so how can we keep this broader perspective as
reflected in the eleven dispositions (see
above)?
08
Brent G. Wilson
Similarly, human questions of ethics,
social justice, moral obligation, politics, etc. are usually
thought to be outside the scope of science - yet they are definitely
relevant to designing and sustaining learning environments. How
should these various concerns be organized and prioritized with
respect to learning environments - and how does the scientific
mind accommodate these concerns?
A further broadening of perspective.
A 'scientific' approach to social justice is nearly absurd -
science isn't meant to answer questions of values and justice
and equity - yet these are serious concerns for learning environments,
as perusal of AERA
journals would demonstrate.
09
Brent G. Wilson
Given the rapid rise of neuroscience,
challenging the construct of mind - is 'scientific mind' an oxymoron?
This is mostly a tease - but does
point to this conflict between the two major terms. Is mind real
- or is it just a placeholder we're using until neuroscience
tells us how things really are?
J. Michael Spector
SITUATED RATIONALE
The concept of a scientific
mind is somewhat fuzzy and ill-defined. This creates a challenge
for those who wish to create experiences and activities to foster
the development of a scientific mind. The challenge is determining
whether and to what extent particular experiences and activities
have been worthwhile and productive. In any case, I shall say
briefly how I understand the notion of the scientific mind and
then pose a few questions. In one sense, a scientific mind is
one that is inclined to engage in persistent and disciplined
inquiry with regard to a particular area of investigation or
set of phenomena. In another sense, a scientific mind can be
associated with being a skeptical inquirer--a person who
(a) admits to not knowing or
understanding something,
(b) is convinced that it would
be interesting or important to know and understand, and
(c) is willing to consider alternative
approaches and explanations along the way.
In any case, it is worth noting
that one cannot be scientific in either sense all of the time
about all issues. It then makes sense to think about inclinations,
dispositions, habits of the mind and such. Being scientific is
not a purely cognitive skill--there are attitudes and feelings
and values involved. Before posing questions, I want to remark
on the difference between posing questions and having questions.
Posing a question, which I shall do shortly, involves a linguistic
activity--namely formulating a particular grammatical structure
(e.g., using a question word, reversing the normal subject-verb
order in English and putting a question mark at the end of the
sentence). Posing a question is easily learned and easily accomplished
in most situations. What is the difference between posing and
having? We all pose questions, and we often do so to indicate
a kind of posture or position with regard to an issue. However,
such posed questions often lack the admission of not knowing,
the subsequent process of inquiry, the willingness to explore
alternatives, and so on. Occasionally, someone who poses a question
ends up having a question; having a question is indicated by
inquiry related activities among other things. The original meaning
of 'skeptic' was 'inquirer' - a skeptic is someone who admits
to not knowing but who is engaged in trying to find out. I regard
that as a marker of a scientific mind.
10
J. Michael Spector
What is the role of formulating
questions in developing a scientific mind?
The motivation for this question
should be obvious from my opening remarks. It is clearly important
to formulate questions to guide inquiry--at least on many occasions,
but posing a question is quite different from having a question.
What is the nature of that relationship and is it important in
the development of a scientific mind?
11
J. Michael Spector
How can one assess progress in developing
a scientific mind?
This is a central challenge indicated
in my opening statement. The notion of the scientific mind is
somewhat vague and people cannot be expected to exhibit the qualities
of a scientific mind all of the time on every issue or problem.
How then can we determine that educational and professional activities
are contributing in a meaningful way to the development of a
scientific mind?
12
J. Michael Spector
To what extent does and should society
value the development of scientific minds in its youth?
I ask this question because an unstated
assumption is often that being more scientific is highly desirable--that
is certainly an assumption that I often make. Is this assumption
shared widely throughout society and should it be widely shared?
13
J. Michael Spector
To what extent have we gotten beyond
Bacon's idols on account of scientific progress?
Sir Francis Bacon identified
four idols (undesirable ways of thinking) that he thought science
could and should address:
(a) idols of the tribe--false
conceptions due to human nature,
(b) idols of the cave--false
conceptions due to upbringing and individual bias,
(c) idols of the marketplace--the
truth they are telling is simply the truth that is selling, and
(d) idols of the theatre--false
notions based on sophistry and rhetoric without a foundation
in evidence or facts.
Are these idols still bedeviling
society some 400 years or so later? What can we then say about
the nature of scientific progress and the development of the
scientific mind?
Michael Hannafin
SITUATED RATIONALE
Not provided.
14
Michael Hannafin
To what extent does (or should)
epistemology influence teaching and learning inquiry and practice?
Researchers and practitioners espouse
varied beliefs as to the nature, role and meaning of epistemology.
The debate in both the study and practice of teaching and learning
reflects these differences (e.g., relativist v. constructivist),
but does it really matter? Why? When? For whom? In some cases,
epistemological differences are reflected in research and practice
processes and activities; in others, they are not. And, in many
cases where differences are explicit, neither research nor teaching
practices have demonstrated improvements or benefits over instances
where epistemology was not evident. At the end of the day, what
has the focus on understanding and examining epistemological
differences contributed to research and practice? How can (or
should) differences be reflected in inquiry and practice?
15
Michael Hannafin
If we know so much about differences
between expert and novice reasoning, why have we struggled to
apply this in the design of learning environments?
A great deal of research has been
published on differences between novices and experts. This literature,
however, has been more helpful in characterizing differences
between experts and novices than in supporting the novice in
developing and refining expertise. We need to consider whether
expert-novice research can be applied to design, or is more accurately
viewed as contributing basic research to our understanding.
16
Michael Hannafin
Can scientific understanding of
human learning really be advanced through research that lacks
a compelling scientific base?
The divisions between classical
empiricists and relativists are among the most contentious of
issues related to inquiry. This is often evident in the strident
criticisms of empiricists who contend that relativist inquiry
lacks the rigor, grounding and discipline to be considered 'scientific,'
and thus does not adhere to or comply with the standards of acceptable
research.
Charles Reigeluth
SITUATED RATIONALE
The abstract for this session
highlights the construct of mental states or mindsets that are
comprised of a broad mix of interacting competencies, attitudes,
values, and metacognitive abilities (to which I would add understandings
or mental models). They determine, in a holistic manner, our
ways of being and acting in the world. I believe this is an extremely
important area of inquiry. In my work on paradigm change in public
education systems, I find that the key to success is helping
people to evolve their mindsets about education, which in my
view are related to their broader mindsets about the world.
However, I do not like the focus
on 'the scientific mindset.' I would propose that there is no
such thing. At best I would say there was an industrial-age scientific
mindset, based on the Cartesian or mechanistic view (mindset)
of the world. And there is an information-age scientific mindset,
based on a systems-thinking, chaos-theory, and sciences-of-complexity
view of the world.
I am sure there are many different
kinds of mindsets on many different levels of generality or scope.
On the broadest level of the continuum, I see mindsets based
on Toffler's distinctions among the hunting-and-gathering society,
the agrarian society, the industrial society, and the information
society (with perhaps the spiritual society to come next?). In
particular, I note the mechanistic worldview of the industrial
age and the complex-systemic worldview of the information age.
On the simpler end of the continuum might be something like a
mindset for collaboration as opposed to a mindset of adversarial
nature.
My interests are first in developing
a better understanding of what a mindset or worldview is and
second in developing a better understanding of how to help someone
develop or change a given mindset or worldview. Therefore, I
offer two groups of questions.
17
Charles Reigeluth
What is the best way to develop
an understanding of what a mindset is? Does it entail picking
one particular mindset and dissecting it to see what parts it
is made of? Does it also entail developing an understanding of
how all those parts interact with and influence each other?
How much is it likely to differ
from one kind of mindset to another?
Understand what 'mindset' is.
18
Charles Reigeluth
What is the best way to develop
an understanding of how to help someone develop (or change) a
mindset? Is there possibly a 'high leverage' aspect of the mindset
that once changed, makes it relatively easy to change the others?
How strong a role does emotion play compared to logic? How much
is it likely to differ from one kind of mindset to another? What
role might vicarious experience through a movie (re: Bandura's
Social Learning Theory) play to help develop or change a particular
mindset?
Understand how to develop or
change a particular mindset.
Glen Bull
SITUATED RATIONALE
I work in the context of a multidisciplinary
unit situated in a teacher education program. A number of the
issues posed by Jan Visser (Questions 1-5 above) speak directly
to the challenge of preparing science educators. Mike Spector
asks whether society values development of scientific minds in
its youth (Question 12). For purposes of discussion, I make the
assumption that policy makers, at least, place a high value on
development of the scientific mind. This leads directly to the
question of how progress in development of a scientific mind
might best be assessed (Question 11), which, in turn, leads to
the issues under consideration.
19
Glen Bull
To what extent is it possible to
understand the methods employed by scientists and apply them
to science education?
When experts are asked to describe
their work, they often omit as many as 70% of the steps involved.
Often they are not able to consciously access the specific processes
followed. In some instances, they may reconstruct a story that
differs substantially from actual practice. The narrative that
results may not be an accurate guide for science education. David
Feldon, for example, notes that research on knowledge elicitation
and STEM experts cognition indicates that self-reports
of problem solving processes are usually incomplete or inaccurate.
(Feldon, in press). Techniques such as cognitive task analysis
may offer a lens for identifying the actual methods employed.
20
Glen Bull
To what extent do science teachers
employ the methods and concepts taught in their pre-service preparation?
Development of the scientific mind
in formal education rests on the assumption that teachers will
employ the methods and concepts presented in their pre-service
preparation. Evidence exists that this is not always the case.
The understanding of the scientific mind developed through informal
experiences over the course of a lifetime can be resistant to
change. Randy Bell notes that future teachers may present the
desired answers on formal examinations, but revert to their prior
understandings in actual teaching practice after graduation.
21
Glen Bull
What factors enforce current compartmentalization
of disciplines, and to what extent does this impede development
of the scientific mind?
Jan Visser notes teaching occurs
in accordance with set curricula that are highly compartmentalized
in disciplines, often as the product of prevailing ideologies
(Question 3). This raises the question of potential benefits
of integrated learning, as well as barriers to integration. Some
of the barriers may be historical, while others may be cultural.
An amalgamated view of initial contributions made
(to inform the next
round of deliberations)*
There is apparent consensus
regarding the need to consider the implications of learning
at higher levels of complexity, particularly to go beyond
the level of compartmentalized competencies. None of us seems
to deny the value of the competency based approaches as such,
but we seem to agree that this is but one important level of
consideration and analysis. More is required, particularly considering
the increasingly complex problems individuals and humanity at
large are facing. Evidence in this regard dates back to the Gagné-Merrill
paper in ETR&D on integrative goals. It has started to become
addressed in Van Merriënboer's 4C/ID model regarding whole-task
approaches to training complex problem solving skills. Invoking
the development of mind in a lifelong learning perspective
elevates the problem to a level beyond the accomplishment of
individual training outcomes and educational program goals. We
encounter different levels of complexity in life and develop
learning behaviors at different levels of complexity. Thus, from
a research and development perspective, one wishes to be pragmatic,
choosing perspectives, theoretical models, modes of reasoning
as well as devising and exploring alternative interventions based
on the level of complexity at hand.
There seems to be diversity
of thought regarding the idea of 'mind'. For some it is
too reminiscent of Cartesian dualism and notions of mind that
do not explicitly acknowledge the material. While the study of
mind makes great progress, particularly thanks to the development
of the neurosciences, we must cautiously clarify our position(s).
Do we agree that 'mind' comprises a broad mix of interacting
competencies, attitudes, values, metacognitive abilities, and
understandings or mental models (see session abstract and Reigeluth
rationale)?
Divergence seems to be even
greater regarding the notion of 'scientific mind'. For
some, 'science' evokes images of narrow-minded pursuit of knowledge,
based on materialist assumptions, devoid of any concern with
values and issues such as social justice, a picture still exacerbated
by reminders of industrial age mechanistic worldviews. But others
might claim the opposite, recognizing that different views of
science, such as Bronowski's, have long existed and been espoused
by those who represent true scientific development. Besides,
new theoretical frameworks, such as advanced in Latour's work
on 'reassembling the social', are emerging. Again, there is a
need to clarify our position(s) and view(s) regarding what it
means to have a scientific mind. Several of the available contributions
have started doing so already.
Related to how we define the
'scientific mind', the question must be raised (as Spector does)
how its presence in citizens, particularly the younger generation,
is valued by society. In other words: What is the relationship
between the development of the scientific mind and the realization
of a society's aspirations? Looking back, and inspired by
concerns expressed in the past (e.g. Francis Bacon [see Q13]),
have we made progress? If not, what has gone wrong? If progress
was made, what has been particularly helpful? How may answers
to the above questions vary across and within cultures and societies?
Related to the above is also
the question regarding how our worldviews have changed,
and will be changing further, over the ages (Reigeluth rationale).
It is probably a fair assumption that the mindsets we develop
cannot be seen in isolation of the worldviews we hold. This calls
for further exploration. What do we mean by worldview? How is
it distinct from mind or mindset? What kind of relationship(s)
are there between the two categories?
Another question relating to
the definition of the scientific mind regards the extent to which
the scientific mind is a function of awareness of or familiarity
with the epistemological underpinnings of the scientific
enterprise. Is epistemology important at all? What else is crucial?
Is the scientific mind simply a complex tool that helps us employ
the methods that scientists employ? But then, what about the
assertion of many of the (often best) scientists that there is
no such thing as 'the scientific method'? What components/dimensions
of the scientific mind can be identified? What is the mix of
rationality and emotion? What about the 11 dispositions
mentioned in the introduction to this page? How distinct
is the scientific mindset from other mindsets? What overlap
exists between different mindsets? At a more comprehensive level:
What does it mean to come to know in today's world and
what does that imply for learning and cognition; for the day-to-day
life of individual citizens; and for decision making about humanity's
destiny at large?
If it is agreed that there is
a need to elevate our thinking about education to levels that
comprise more explicitly and more predominantly the human ability
to act and learn in complex domains, and assuming that the idea
of 'mind' can be constructed in ways that allow us to reflect
more adequately on the issues at stake, how can we operationalize
this construct in the sense that it can be used to measure progress
in a human being's development? What observables can be identified
that can guide educators, designers of learning conditions and
environments, and policy makers?
Questions can be raised as well
regarding what of the available research base is relevant
to the problems at hand for this conversation. Specifically,
does research on the difference between novice and expert
behavior and reasoning provide any useful insight?
Finally, considering the potential
relevance of the formal educational environment (no doubt in
addition to numerous informal settings) for the development of
the scientific mind, how should we educate teachers; what
attributes of the teaching/learning environment help teachers
become better facilitators of the development of the scientific
mind; and what kind of curriculum reform is required to
overcome the kind of disciplinary compartmentalization
that suggests that the development of particular mindsets is
the exclusive responsibility of teachers specialized in specific
content areas? Notwithstanding the importance of thinking beyond
disciplinary boundaries, however, there is also the exceedingly
important question about how the proper study of science,
with its emphasis on rigorous disciplined inquiry and deep understanding
of the workings of nature, can once again be restored as a serious
focus area of the formal curriculum.
*
Prepared by Jan Visser, with thanks to feedback received from
Yusra Laila Visser and Michael Spector.
Remarks
on Building the Scientific Mind for AECT 2009 - Michael Spector
October 21, 2009
The large issue we are addressing
is this: How best to design curricula, courses and learning activities
so that they actively and systematically contribute to the progressive
development of higher order thinking and reasoning skills required
for sustained scientific inquiry. While we have not precisely
defined the target, we are calling it the 'scientific mind'.
We seem to have accepted a number of assumptions:
The scientific mind is a set
of mental states or mindsets, cognitive skills, dispositions
and probably more that enable a person to confront and work effectively
to resolve complex problems (e.g., designing experiments, determining
alternative solution approaches, developing productive explanations,
and so on);
Other mindsets are possible
(e.g., the artistic mind, the political mind, the religious mind,
etc.) - presumably, other mindsets might share some of the characteristics
of the scientific mind;
It is highly desirable from
the perspective of life in 21st century society to foster the
development of the scientific mind - not necessarily to the exclusion
of the development of other mindsets;
Challenging problems confronted
by scientists in the 21st century often cross traditional discipline
boundaries and typically require the coordinated efforts of teams
of people with various backgrounds and training; and,
Holistic approaches to developing
the skills and dispositions characteristic of the scientific
mind are required.
Not everyone agrees with the
terminology or concepts involved in the description of the scientific
mind. Some object to the fuzziness of 'mind' and 'mental states',
and some object to a narrow or materialistic interpretation of
'science'. Admittedly, both 'mind' and 'science' are broad and
somewhat fuzzy concepts. Rather than revisit the conceptual struggles
of philosophers in the last couple millennia, it might be possible
to take a pragmatic approach and examine what it is that people
who are called scientists actually do and how they think about
the problems they actually confront.
As an example, consider an astronomer
who is investigating the effects of sunspots on weather patterns.
The astronomer must be able to identify and analyze weather patterns;
this probably requires some communication with a meteorologist,
for example - a scientist with a different background and with
different training. Of course the astronomer will communicate
with other astronomers who have studied solar activity. Eventually,
the astronomer might formulate a hypothesis with regard to specific
sunspot activity and an effect on the weather. To test the hypothesis,
the astronomer must then record sunspot activity. How to do that?
Well, one way might be to use the solar observatory in New Mexico
and schedule a series of recorded observations of sunspots using
the telescope and cameras available there. This now requires
some communication and coordination with computer scientists
and technicians to prepare and program the telescope and cameras.
If new or modified devices are required, various engineers might
be involved. Once the data is recorded, it must be analyzed;
that analysis might require the assistance of a highly skilled
mathematician. Then, data pertaining to the weather must be collected
and analyzed. Perhaps a collaborating meteorologist will be enlisted
to help with this activity.
In the course of such activity,
the astronomer will be engaged in many different kinds of activities
and very likely will be working in concert with others. To truly
understand what such a scientist does and how he/she thinks,
one would have to observe the specific activities and ask many
questions. It is not likely that such an investigation into how
that astronomer works and thinks would leave a neat and tidy
picture of an idealized scientific method, but it might reveal
qualities of working and thinking that also occur in the everyday
activities of other scientists. Such qualities or patterns based
on what scientists do and how they think about the problems with
which they are engaged could then be used as the basis for what
could be called a 'scientific mind'.
Conducting such investigations
into how scientists work and think is not a novel idea. Indeed
many papers and books have been written on this topic. Some (e.g.,
George Johnson's How Scientists Think: Key Experiments in Genetics
- 1995, Brown Publishing) focus primarily on the science involved
in critical experiments - that focus would be overly narrow for
the activity described above. Others focus on the creative problem
solving that led to important discoveries (e.g., How Scientists
Think: Fostering Creativity in Problem Solving - see Topics in
Cognitive Science, 1(4), 730-757 & http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2009/09/090921162150.htm).
However, focusing only on the science surrounding important discoveries
would also be overly narrow, in my opinion. Not very many scientists
make important discoveries, but they do work on meaningful problems
and contribute to the progress of science. There are also books
and papers on how scientists ought to think, and some of these
are based on empirical research and not merely a recasting of
the idealized scientific method found in textbooks.
All such works should be considered
as we try to develop a clear sense of what a scientific mind
is [or is like]. Case histories of important discoveries, interviews
with leading scientists, interviews with and observations of
scientists working at all levels in a variety of research settings,
anecdotes about failed experiments and accidental discoveries,
and more could comprise a kind of ethnography of scientific minds
at work.
I believe that such investigations
into the nature and qualities of scientific activity should continue
and could be used to further refine and inform the concept of
a scientific mind. This is no small undertaking, however. In
a society that is addicted to quick fixes and easy solutions,
such an approach is not likely to be highly valued. Still, rather
than argue over words and to avoid adopting and advocating positions
that might not result in desired outcomes, we might do well to
encourage such pragmatic research into the natural epistemology
of scientific inquiry. What do everyday scientists do everyday,
and how do they think about the problems they encounter?
I feel somewhat disoriented
approaching the topic of Scientific Mind as it relates to the
design of learning environments. The construct may be found relevant
at two levels:
Creating instruction within
K20 programs targeting the scientific mind as learning outcomes
for students
Encouraging teachers and learning-environment
designers to a adopt a scientific-mind stance as they create
courses and programs
Science is usually seen as a
core subject for K12 curriculum, then as an important foundation
and common specialization in higher education - so I see the
relevance there.
I struggle, though, privileging the scientific mind above the
other kinds of minds Mike Spector refers to. As a thought exercise
it's fruitful to explore the construct of scientific mind, but
what about the engineering mind? The designer mind? The artistic
or craftsman mind? The public-servant mind? Consistent with my
pluralistic tendencies, I keep thinking of who's not invited
to the table, whose voice is not heard in the conversation. So
I do sense a confusion or tension between focusing on one construct,
even if we stipulate (as in Mike's assumptions) that a holistic
approach is good. The holistic conversation - with everybody
participating - will likely yield the best overall results.
This ambivalence seems to be inherent in the Scientific Mind
framework. I really resonate to the Eleven Dispositions:
The spirit of inquiry.
The spirit of collaboration.
The quest for beauty (harmony,
parsimony, wholeness).
The desire to understand and
do so profoundly.
The creative spirit.
The urge to be critical.
The spirit to transcend.
The spirit of building on prior
knowledge.
The search for unity.
The building of the story of
human knowledge and ability.
The spirit of construction.
They do have an inclusive or
holistic quality that is very appealing. But then, what would
happen if we put a different label on the same list, say:
Dispositions of the Artistic Mind
The spirit of inquiry.
The spirit of collaboration.
The quest for beauty (harmony,
parsimony, wholeness).
The desire to understand and
do so profoundly.
The creative spirit.
The urge to be critical.
The spirit to transcend.
The spirit of building on prior
knowledge.
The search for unity.
The building of the story of
human knowledge and ability.
The spirit of construction.
Which of the dispositions look
out of place for the artist? I can't see one. Or the designer?
I take this dexterity to be an overall good sign - we are indeed
being broad and inclusive in developing the construct.
Now to the question - are these values and dispositions current
manifest in practice? If I were to rank them in order of the
attention given among educators, it might look something like
this:
The spirit of building on prior
knowledge.
The building of the story of
human knowledge and ability.
The desire to understand and
do so profoundly.
The spirit of construction.
The spirit of inquiry.
The search for unity.
The urge to be critical.
The spirit of collaboration.
The quest for beauty (harmony,
parsimony, wholeness).
The creative spirit.
The spirit to transcend.
But my personal reaction in
doing that exercise is how wonderful each of the dispositions
is - and how we need more of all eleven! Perhaps less (proportionally)
of other dispositions common in education like:
Fitting in and conforming with
the group
Being and staying cool
Playing the game
Following directions
Respecting authority
Working hard
Delaying gratification
All of these are valuable too,
of course - but they are over-represented in our current learning
environments. As are:
Finding your passion and pursuing
it
Relying on yourself
Taking responsibility
I hope I am not diluting the
conversation by looking at other dispositions and values. Rather
I am trying to place the given construct into our familiar contexts
of practice. In that context I do find them valuable and worth
more attention in both theory and practice. So in spite of my
quibbles about the two key terms of Scientific Mind, the framework
transcends these terms and rises to a level approaching the holism
(and perhaps pluralism) that I so value.
Some notes
in the margin of An amalgamated view of initial contributions
made - Jan Visser
October 23, 2009
On learning
In my work over the past ten
years, I have increasingly looked at the world of learning through
a wide-angle lens, bringing within view not only the traditional
concerns that drive human learning, but also the broad issues
with which we, as human individuals and as a species, interact.
I'm referring here to issues that--at the current juncture in
evolutionary history--must be considered of key relevance to
the survival of our species and its planetary habitat. They include,
but are not limited to, such matters as our ability to live in
harmony with our fellow human beings and our natural (biological
as well as physical) environment; the shared use of our planet's
resources in ways that are responsive to the needs of future
generations in an evolutionary perspective; and our responsibilities
to safeguard the cultural and intellectual heritage of the past,
enriching it with our own contributions, offering it to future
generations who can interact with and build upon it. As a concrete
example, it is difficult to explore what is involved in, say,
'learning for sustainable futures' if we can only do so in terms
of a set of competencies.
In the recent past, problems
such as the ones just mentioned could perhaps still be thought
of and dealt with at the level of relatively isolated communities
and societies. Not so any longer. Over the time span of half
a century we have rapidly become a global community of close
to 6.7 billion people whose individual lives touch upon the lives
of all others. That change in perceived reality has altered my
view of human learning. I no longer see learning as a series
of predominantly deliberate, time-bound, efforts, distributed
along the lifespan, aiming at becoming better at doing things
for particular purposes as expressed in neatly defined acquired
competencies. Rather, I look at it as a continuous disposition
towards uninterrupted dialogue with our human, social, biological
and natural environment so as to become better and better at
constructively interacting with change--change that we collaboratively
create and to which we must at the same time continually adapt
as it happens.(1) It's a
view of learning that encompasses past, more limited views, while
seeking to capture the reality of learning at higher levels of
complexity. In the formal context of training and education one
will often choose to look at learning in terms of traditional
definitions that look for changed behavior expressed in terms
of specific competencies. However, such definitions are less
adequate when dealing with more holistic ways of being and acting.
Hence my desire to identify units of analysis at higher levels
of complexity. However, the problem lies in operationalizing
such constructs in the sense that they can be used to measure
progress in a human being's development? Identifying relevant
observables of the presence and growth of the scientific mind
is thus a key challenge.
On mind
I use the word 'mind' in a down-to-earth
fashion. The term existed before we had neuroscience and before
we knew much about the brain. Now that we know a little more
it is clear that we must relate the meaning of mind to functions
of the brain. Besides, we know that we must conceive of the brain
as an organ that is integral to the human body as a whole and
to the faculties of that body to sense and interact with the
environment. The Anglo-Saxon origin of the word, 'gemynd,' simply
means memory. It's perhaps not a bad idea to think of mind as
memory, provided we recognize that memory as it pertains to humans
is not the same as memory in the context of computer science.
We shape our memory--and, as Susan Greenfield (2) asserts,
personalize our physical brain--in accordance with what we decide
to do with our experience along the lifespan. Thus we develop,
consciously and sub-consciously, dispositions, underscored by
acquired competencies, to approach the world in particular ways.
Because, as we now know, mind
is not separate from brain; brain is both cognition and emotion;
brain is integral to body and sensory experience; and sensory
experience makes us consciously (cognitively and emotionally)
part of the universe, there is no need for Descartes bashing.
This eminent scientist and philosopher made great contributions
without which we would not have advanced to where we are now.
We can look back in gratitude to those, like Ptolemy, Newton,
Descartes, and Skinner, who saw the world their way and who,
by expressing their views, helped us make the next steps. The
study of the history of how we developed intellectually may help
us understand better the relative validity and importance of
our own insights. It may equally help us not to lose sight of
our own ignorance.
On science
I have no problem with science
and scientists. I am one myself and feel passionately part of
the scientific community. Science is perhaps best described in
a recent lecture by Lee Smolin (3) as "a
community based process, premised on ethical standards, that
leads from shared evidence to shared conclusions."
On the scientific mind
Because of the community-based
nature of the scientific undertaking, I value the community spirit
present in what I see as 'the scientific mind.' There is no doubt
such community spirit also in other areas of human undertaking,
such as in religion and the arts, in politics and among poets,
but all those communities are different and unique in their own
right. None of the alternatives I can think of seems to be defined
by Smolin's description above. Hence, I feel it makes sense to
identify different broad dimensions of mental development.
I agree with Brent Wilson that
there is tremendous overlap between mindsets, just as there is
tremendous overlap between all members of the human species.
Yet scientists are different from politicians, poets and those
who shape religious experience, even though some scientists may
adopt roles in such other areas. The biologist Leo Vroman (of
Dutch origin, later naturalized in America) is a renowned poet
(I count him among the best known to me worldwide); the Jesuit
trained geologist and paleontologist Pierre Teilhard de Chardin
made important contributions to paleontology, in addition to
experimenting with worldviews that, as he saw it, reconciled
evolving scientific insights with religious experience; and some
scientists even err into the realm of politics. This would likely
not happen if the 11 dispositions were exclusive to the scientific
mind. What makes the difference may be determined by how we prepare
the dish and by how and in which order we mix in the different
ingredients. There will likely not be a single way of preparing
a delicious and healthy dish, but there are many subtle ways
in which the result of our cuisine may be a disaster or simply
fall short of the excellence to which we aspire.
While there is value in the
notion of 'scientific method', the notion of 'THE scientific
method' is unhelpful. Elements such as those suggested by Mike
Spector--skepticism (in the sense of thoughtfulness), disciplined
exploration, peer scrutiny and validation, etc.--are more important
than following set procedures. In that sense the scientific mind
is distinct from the bureaucratic mind and more aligned with
the artistic mind and the poetic mind.
Finally, we may not want to
equate the idea of scientific mind too narrowly with what we
perceive to be the mindset of those who do science for a profession.
Most people for whose education we care will not become professional
scientists, professional poets, or professional designers. Yet,
they may benefit from having developed propensities and abilities
of mind aligned with the value systems that underlie science,
poetry and design.
My suggestion to investigate
what the learning sciences may have to say about the development
of the scientific mind is an arbitrary choice, biased by my own
passions and interests. There is no doubt equal value in engaging
in similar explorations of relevant additional mindsets.
---
Notes and references:
(1)
The formal definition refers
to "the disposition of human beings, and of the social entities
to which they pertain, to engage in continuous dialogue with
the human, social, biological and physical environment, so as
to generate intelligent behavior to interact constructively with
change" (Visser , J. (2001). Integrity, completeness and
comprehensiveness of the learning environment: Meeting the basic
learning needs of all throughout life. In D. N. Aspin, J. D.
Chapman, M. J. Hatton and Y. Sawano (Eds), International Handbook
of Lifelong Learning (pp. 447-472). Dordrecht, The Netherlands:
Springer).
(2) Greenfield,
S. (2000). The private life of the brain: Emotions, consciousness,
and the self. New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
The panel session (see photos
above and below) started off with a 15-minute presentation by
Jan Visser, supported by a slideshow on Building
the Scientific Mind: Learning for a complex world(PDF
version). It was followed by brief comments by the other panelists
and concluded with a half-hour animated doialogue between and
among panelists and members of the audience.
From left to right: Charles
Reigeluth, Glen Bull, Brent Wilson, and Michael Spector